Creative ideas, of course, must be original in some way. But they also aim to provide some sort of value to people, whether that value is aesthetic, entertainment, practical, or financial
In any creative pursuit, there is a delicate balance between novelty and convention. Creative ideas, of course, must be original in some way. But they also aim to provide some sort of value to people, whether that value is aesthetic, entertainment, practical, or financial. Therefore, would-be creatives face a dilemma: in deciding where to dig for that next great idea, how much should they prioritize originality versus areas of proven success?
Sometimes we get that balance wrong. Consider a recent review of a Michelin-starred restaurant name Bros. The popular food blogger, Geraldine DeRuiter, describes an excruciating three-hour affair. Twenty-seven courses of what sound more like exhibits at a haunted house than elevated fine dining. Some highlights include an oyster loaf that “tasted like Newark airport,” a squirt of gelee infused with droplets of meat molecules, a tasting of 12 different kinds of foam, “frozen air” and several pieces of flavoured paper. The coup de grace? A dollop of citrus-flavoured foam served in a plaster cast of the chef’s mouth. Absent utensils, diners were instructed to lick the foam out of the plaster cast prompting a scene that the blogger described as something “stolen from an eastern European horror film.”
In the case of Bros. the mistake is rather clear: in striving to create a dining experience that is totally unique and one-of-a-kind, the chef seems to have forgotten the most important element of all: making the food actually taste good. But it turns out that this mistake is a lot more common than you might think. Indeed, when most people think do something creative, their mind races to what’s never been done before: ‘Where is the wide-open space, the untouched soil?’ In turn, they begin digging in areas that seem totally new. But what the science suggests is that rather than looking to what has never been done before, it can be far more advantageous to consider the areas in which successful ideas have been found in the past. In other words, if people’s intuitive ratio is that creative ideas should be mostly novel and only a bit conventional, the science suggests the opposite: they should seek topics and pursuits that are mostly conventional and only a bit novel.
In some of my own research on this topic, my colleagues and I have investigated just how prevalent this bias toward originality might be. We looked at a domain in which most people regard themselves as quite skilled: making sandwiches. Participants in these studies (we called them, ‘chefs’) were asked to brainstorm new sandwich recipes for a chain of sandwich shops. One group of chefs was asked to design a tasty sandwich, while another group was asked to design a sandwich that was both creative and tasty. The chefs were told that they would earn bonuses based on how appealing their sandwich recipes were to others.
Before we passed off the recipes for evaluation, we asked the chefs a bit about their process. How much had they focused on making the sandwich original? How much had they focused on taste? As you might expect, chefs in the creative and tasty condition said they prioritized originality more than chefs in the tasty condition. However, when it came to taste, all chefs said that tastiness was their primary criterion, regardless of which prompt they received. In other words, all of the chefs told us that they cared primarily about taste, and those who were asked to focus on creativity did not perceive any tradeoff between the originality of their sandwich and the tastiness of the sandwich.
[This article has been reprinted, with permission, from Rotman Management, the magazine of the University of Toronto's Rotman School of Management]